At one point in my Cognitive Science/Philosophy courses, we talked a bit about contextualism about language, which is the idea that we critically rely on various contexts to determine the meaning of a sentence. For example, if I say “I went to the bank yesterday”, the sentence itself is perfectly compatible with my going to that place where I keep my money or to the place beside the river. For the most part, we get the determination right, but most interestingly to me are the cases where we in fact get that spectacularly wrong. In the case where I first heard about this, for example, in the example everyone in the room thought that the lecturer meant that the person should get on the desk, instead of looking for something that they could use on the desk. There are entire genres of comedy built entirely around someone failing to parse the right meaning out of a sentence, and having hilarity ensue. So we find that our ability to disambiguate words is both massively successful and shockingly terrible at times. What explains this ability?
To me, the main clue starts from the psychological process of “priming”. Essentially, this is the process where if we are exposed to, say, a word that is related to another word in a list that we’ve already recently processed, we process that word faster than we would otherwise. So, for example, if you’re reading a list of words and come across the word “Doctor” and then not too much later come across the word “Nurse”, you process “Nurse” faster and easier than you would if you hadn’t come across it beforehand. This is hard to explain.
Being someone from both a philosophical and a computing background, I do have a suggestion for what could be going on here. In general, it seems to me that what we probably have is a combination of time-saving techniques that are common in computer science when loading time is an issue. First, if it is common for a bunch of things to all be referenced together, instead of loading precisely the part you need and no more and then immediately loading the other parts, you load the whole thing into memory and use it. If you don’t use all of it, you don’t lose much because the problem is the initial loading and seeking out the object you’re looking for, not loading the individual parts of it. The second thing is to store things in memory that you have recently used because you’re likely to want to use it again in a short period of time, which is often implemented by or called “cacheing”. There are a number of Cognitive Science AI theories that rely on storing and loading objects and contexts instead of, say, simply words, so all we need to do, then, is add cacheing.
I’ve written a little program to play with cacheing to show how priming could work using it. I won’t reproduce the program here because HTML wants to ignore leading spaces and Python critically depends on leading spaces, so it’s a lot of work to put a program here, but in general what the program does is set up a number of lists that contain various characters that have various traits. For my demo, I created one with David Eddings characters, one with Persona characters, and one with other characters. The lists are as follows:
[Kalten, Sparhawk, Sephrenia, Ulath, Ehlana]
[Akihiko, Dojima, Yukari, Junpei, Naoto, Adachi, Yu, Mitsuru]
[Sherlock Holmes]
I then set up some matching criteria that you can ask the system to look for. You can look to see if the character is a Knight, is Male, is a Fictional Character, Carries a Sword, is a Detective, or is a Video Game Character. And you can ask for multiple criteria to be matched. For example, this was my first criteria:
print(matchMemoryElement([“Video Game Character”,”Carries A Sword” ]))
And given the lists above, the first one that it finds is Junpei.
So what if I run that search and then run another one looking for an Eddings Character. Note that since I randomize the lists every time (to allow me to get odd results without having to plan things out), the lists on this run start as follows:
[Kalten, Ehlana, Ulath, Sparhawk, Sephrenia]
[Dojima, Akihiko, Naoto, Junpei, Yukari, Adachi, Yu, Mitsuru]
[Sherlock Holmes]
And the results are:
[Sephrenia, Dojima, Akihiko, Naoto, Junpei]
Junpei
[Sephrenia, Dojima, Akihiko, Naoto, Junpei]
Sephrenia
So we still find Junpei for the first criteria, as he’s still the first person in the lists that is both a video game character and carries a sword. But how come I found Sephrenia first for the Eddings character? She’s the last in the list; shouldn’t I have found Kalten first?
The reason is that 5 element list that is printed out before the answer. That’s a cache, where I store the last five elements I’ve processed in case I need them again so I don’t have to go back to the lists. In this case, I parsed through all of the Eddings characters list, and then only got to the fourth element in the list of Persona characters before finding one, and then when I tried to match the second set of criteria it looked in the cache, found Sephrenia, and gave me that one … which would have been embarrassing if I was really looking for Kalten.
Let’s see what happens when instead of looking for an Eddings character, I look for a detective. The lists this time are:
[Ehlana, Sparhawk, Kalten, Sephrenia, Ulath]
[Yu, Junpei, Adachi, Yukari, Akihiko, Dojima, Naoto, Mitsuru]
[Sherlock Holmes]
And the results are:
[Sparhawk, Kalten, Sephrenia, Ulath, Yu]
Yu
[Sephrenia, Ulath, Yu, Junpei, Adachi]
Adachi
This time, there wasn’t a detective in the cache when it started, so it had to go back to the list to look for one, and ended up with Adachi.
Caches save loading time, because if you’ve already loaded an object and might use it again you might be able to get it from the cache without having to load any objects again. Also, despite the fact that the behaviour looks intelligent, it’s really quite simple, as all it does is store what you’ve loaded. Simple caches have no idea what you might load next, and even don’t have to intentionally cache in case that object might be needed again. All you need is a kind of white board that you just don’t erase, and a system that always looks on the white board first, and if nothing is there it erases some space and writes something else down. It’s a system that a brain could indeed implement by accident just by dealing with activation potentials. And yet, it has a lot of power to explain things like priming and contextualization of language processing. I hope to delve more into this if I have some time, but for now this ought to do to give a quick idea of the potential of cacheing for AI.
Tags: AI
July 22, 2015 at 3:02 pm |
Roy Harris’ Signs of Writing is an interesting take on reading, legibility and the semiotics of text interpretation. His conclusion being that meaning is determined by context and that as we get more context we become faster at understanding meaning.
July 14, 2016 at 5:26 am |
look up the [sourcecode] wordpress tag. makes it easy to post source code nicely with no effort whatsoever
July 14, 2016 at 7:16 am |
I’ll try to remember that for the next time I add some code here.
…
I have WAY too many things I want to do [grin].
February 20, 2019 at 4:56 am |
[…] was a lot of discussions about that in the comments, and that made me want to do AI again after it being a … few years since my last attempt. And, of course, I clearly have lots of time to spare and no other projects that I want to look at […]